Skip to main content
Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Arch Gen Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2013 May 8.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3647246
NIHMSID: NIHMS452834
PMID: 23117638

Amygdala volume changes with posttraumatic stress disorder in a large case-controlled veteran group

Rajendra A. Morey, M.D., M.S.,a,b,c,d Andrea L. Gold, M.S.,e Kevin S. LaBar, Ph.D.,a,b,d Shannon K. Beall, B.S.,a,c Vanessa M. Brown, B.A.,a,c Courtney C. Haswell, M.S.,a,c Jessica D. Nasser,a,c Mid-Atlantic MIRECC Workgroup, H. Ryan Wagner, Ph.D.,b,c and Gregory McCarthy, Ph.D.c,e

Abstract

Context

Smaller hippocampal volumes are well established in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), but the relatively few studies of amygdala volume in PTSD have produced equivocal results.

Objective

To assess a large cohort of recent military veterans, with PTSD and trauma-exposed controls, with sufficient power to definitively assess the effect of PTSD on volumetric changes in the amygdala and hippocampus, as well as the contribution of illness duration, trauma load, and depressive symptoms.

Design

Case-controlled design with structural MRI and clinical diagnostic assessments. Important potential confounds of alcohol use, depression, and medication use were statistically controlled.

Setting

Durham VA Medical Center located in central North Carolina (USA) in proximity to major military bases.

Patients

Ambulatory patients (n=200), recruited from a registry of military service members and veterans serving after 11-September 2001, consisting of a group with current PTSD (n=99) and trauma-exposed comparison group without PTSD (n=101).

Main Outcome measure

Amygdala and hippocampus volumes computed from automated segmentation of high-resolution structural MRI acquired at 3 Tesla.

Results

Smaller volume was demonstrated in the PTSD compared to non-PTSD groups for the left amygdala (p = .002), right amygdala (p = .01), and left hippocampus (p = .02), but not for the right hippocampus (p = .25). Amygdala volumes were not associated with PTSD chronicity, trauma load, or severity of depressive symptoms.

Conclusions

These results provide clear evidence of an association between smaller amygdala volume and PTSD. The lack of correlation between trauma load or illness chronicity and amygdala volume suggests that either a smaller amygdala represents a vulnerability to developing PTSD, or the lack of a dose-response relationship with amygdala volume. Our results may trigger a renewed impetus for investigating structural differences in the amygdala, its genetic determinants, its environmental modulators, and the possibility that it reflects an intrinsic vulnerability to PTSD.

INTRODUCTION

The amygdala is perhaps the most strongly implicated brain structure in the pathophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Prevalent models of anxiety have focused on an amygdalocentric neurocircuitry1 that is critical in fear response, conditioning, generalization2,3,4, and facilitates the response to stressful experiences5. Functional MRI studies69 have shown that individuals with PTSD have an exaggerated amygdala response to emotional stimuli when compared to controls. Animal studies have demonstrated changes in amygdala morphology with chronic stress10, evident primarily in the growth of dendritic spines. Experimental studies of amygdala volume in mice and humans have shown an association between smaller amygdala volumes, increased levels of fear conditioning, and an exaggerated glucocorticoid response to stress1113. However, efforts to find evidence of an association between amygdala volume and PTSD in humans have produced equivocal results1415. Our goal was to reinvestigate amygdala volume changes in PTSD by addressing some of the potential methodological issues contributing to inconclusive findings.

The hippocampus has been the overwhelming focus of prior studies of morphological change in PTSD. These studies have demonstrated a clear association between smaller hippocampal volume and PTSD1516. In contrast, the relatively few studies of amygdala volume differences in PTSD have yielded mixed results (see Table 1). Two meta-analyses that included amygdala volumetry showed inconsistent differences between trauma-exposed participants with and without PTSD. The first meta-analysis found lower volumes with small effect sizes17 in both the left (effect size= −.22) and right amygdala (−.18)15, but only after restricting analysis to the subset of studies that produced a homogeneous sample. The second meta-analysis demonstrated only a trend association (p = .06), with a small effect size (Hedges’ g = −.29), between smaller left amygdala volume and PTSD patients as compared to trauma-unexposed healthy controls14. The interpretation of these data is complicated by several methodological issues and limitations. For instance, the first meta-analysis15 included a group with intrusive memories but lacked a diagnosis of PTSD, the use of varied manual segmentation protocols, differences in raters across studies, scanners with lower spatial resolution and field strengths than current standards, heterogeneous trauma type (combat, sexual assault, intimate partner violence, etc.), and inclusion of children18. Although these meta-analyses offer weak support for an association of amygdala volume and PTSD, several individual studies that failed to show significant differences1926 suggest caution in this interpretation (see Table 1). Furthermore, the number of studies reporting negative results may be an underestimation of the actual number given the disincentives for publishing negative results.

Table 1

Table 1a. Structural MRI studies of amygdala volume differences in adults with PTSD*
StudyMR field strength (Tesla)slice thickness (mm)in-plane resolution (mm2)Type of traumaPTSD measurePTSD (n)TEC (n)HC (n)§Met est.29 n Amygdala volume (p)effect size (Cohen’s d)
LeftRightLeftRight
Bonne 2001 1721.5.94Mixed: EDCAPS1027040.16.16.44.43
Bremner 1997 181.53.63child maltreatmentSADS17017121.08.23.62.42
Fennema-Notestine 2002 191.54.94IPVCAPS11111719.06NS/NR−.56−.69
Gilbertson 2002 201.51.5.94combat: VietnamCAPS1223067.90.54−.33−.07
Gurvits 1996 211.51.5.94combat: VietnamCAPS77829.53.07−.57−.44
Lindauer 2004 221.511police officersSCID1414040.25.17.47.56
Lindauer 2005 241.511MixedSCID1814040.85.09.23−.44
Rogers 2009 251.51.5.94Tokyo subwayCAPS (Japanese)916012.05.05.88.64
Wignall 2004 231.511Mixed: EDCAPS1501114.07.37.84.36
Table 1b. Structural MRI studies of amygdala volume differences in pediatric PTSD*
StudyMR field strength (Tesla)slice thickness (mm)in-plane resolution (mm)Type of traumaPTSD measurePTSD (n)TEC (n)HC (n)^Met est.17 n Amygdala volume (p)effect size (Cohen’s d)
LeftRightLeftRight
De Bellis 1999 261.51.5.94 × 1.25child maltreatmentK-SADS4406140NS/NRNS/NR.44.40
De Bellis 2001 161.51.5.94 × 1.25child maltreatmentK-SADS90929NS/NRNS/NR.56.29
De Bellis 2002 271.51.5.94 × 1.25child maltreatmentK-SADS28066783.62.73−.11.08

Abbreviations: CAPS, Clinician-Administered PTSD Scales; ED, emergency department; ES, effect size; est., estimated; HC, healthy control; IPV, intimate partner violence; K-SADS, Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age; NR, not reported; NS, non-significant; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; SADS, Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia; SCID, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM; TEC, trauma-exposed control.

§ For healthy control group, trauma exposure was either not assessed or determined to be absent.
The per group sample size for each study was compared with the sample size estimate based on power=.80, alpha=.05 from Morey et al. 29. A small effect size (based on Woon & Hedges12 meta-analysis) was used for the amygdala sample size criterion of ES=.1, n=783; ES=.2, n=199; ES=.25, n=121; ES=.3, n=90; ES=.35, n=67; ES=.4, n=51; ES=.45, n=40; ES=.5, n=34; ES=.55, n=29; ES=.6, n=24; ES=.65, n=22; ES=.7, n=19; ES=.75, n=17; ES=.8, n=15; ES=.85, n=14; ES=.9, n=12. per group for manual tracing.
For studies with three groups, the p-values and Cohen’s d refer to the PTSD vs. TEC group comparison.
*Segmentation was performed by manual tracing for all studies.

Our study had three main goals. The first was to assess the association of amygdala volume and PTSD in a large sample of trauma-exposed adults. The effect sizes from several prior studies that yielded marginal p-values for amygdala differences2122,24,26 suggest they were underpowered to detect real effects in the population. Therefore, we hypothesized that the PTSD group would show smaller amygdala volume than the trauma-exposed non-PTSD group when powered to adequately control for Type II error. We previously reported27 the sample size required to demonstrate a significant group difference in amygdala volume derived from automated segmentation using published effect sizes15 to be at least 55 subjects per group, which is substantially larger than sample sizes used in prior published studies (see Table 1).

The second goal was to gain empirical evidence that might offer clues about factors contributing to PTSD. For example, if trauma exposure, trauma load, or illness chronicity were correlated with amygdala volume, this would be compatible with either an environmental cause for the volume change, or a pre-existing vulnerability that interacts with environmental factors such as trauma load and illness chronicity. Evidence of an association with PTSD in the absence of an association with trauma exposure would allow for the possibility that smaller amygdala volume may represent a vulnerability to PTSD. We hypothesized that a diagnosis of PTSD would be associated with smaller amygdala volume, but that volume would not be correlated with trauma exposure or illness chronicity based on evidence in animals and humans that smaller amygdalae constitute a risk for heightened fear and stress responses1113.

Our third and final goal was to confirm the established finding of smaller hippocampal volume1415 in our sample of veterans serving after 11-September 2011 using the structural neuroimaging methods we have adopted. These methods included several enhancements such as an automated segmentation approach to better control variability in manual segmentation protocols by human raters, improved signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) provided by 3 Tesla field strength, and higher spatial resolution (1-mm isotropic voxels). For analyses of both structures, we used a multiple regression approach similar to Bremner and colleagues21 to control for variables such as symptoms of depression, trauma load, duration of PTSD, intracranial volume (ICV), age, medication, and alcohol abuse.

METHODS

Participants

Participants (n=200) were recruited between September 2007 and November 2010 from a registry28 of military service members and veterans. All participants served since 11-September 2001 and most (77%) served in the Iraq and/or Afghanistan military conflicts. Participants were screened for inclusion/exclusion criteria based on information available in the registry and from subsequent telephone contact where 85% of potential subjects agreed to participate. Important exclusions included major Axis I diagnosis (other than depression), contraindication to MRI, traumatic brain injury, substance dependence, neurological disorders, and age over 55 years. All participants provided written informed consent to participate in procedures reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Duke University and the Durham VA Medical Center. Participants completed questionnaires assessing depressive symptoms (Beck Depression Inventory-II, BDI-II29), traumatic life events (Traumatic Life Events Questionnaire, TLEQ30), combat exposure (Combat Exposure Scale, CES31), alcohol abuse (Alcohol Use Disorders Test, AUDIT32) current medication use, and were administered (n=191; 96%) the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) to assess comorbid Axis I diagnoses. A summary of participants’ demographic and clinical features (see Table 2), indicates groups were matched for age, gender, and race. The PTSD group had greater trauma and combat exposure, depressive symptoms, alcohol use, and psychotropic medication use. PTSD diagnosis was ascertained with the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale33(CAPS) in 149 (75%) participants, and with the Davidson Trauma Scale34(DTS) in 51 (26%) participants. The participants assessed with the DTS were assigned to a diagnostic group based on a cutoff score of 40 that we have previously reported to have high positive (0.95) and negative predictive value (0.85) as compared to clinician-administered interview in a larger sample of veterans serving after 11-September 2001 from the same registry35. Initial study participants received the DTS before the study team had completed CAPS training. Major depression was diagnosed (SCID) in 27 (14%) PTSD and two (1%) control participants.

Table 2

Demographic and Clinical Information by group

CharacteristicPTSD (n=99)Non-PTSD (n=102)Group Comparison
Age (SD), y38.4 (9.9)37.5 (10.6)t = −.549, p = .58
Gender, No. female (%)20 (20)16 (16)χ 2 = .644, p = .42
Race, # Caucasian (%)49 (49)54 (53)χ 2 = .239, p = .67
Beck Depression Inventory, mean (SD)19.3 (11.0)5.56 (5.6)t = −11.2, p <.001
Combat Exposure Scale, mean (SD)15.6 (10.1)8.64 (9.9)t = −4.89, p <.001
Trauma Life Events Questionnaire, mean (SD)14.6 (11.4)7.41 (8.7)t = −5.03, p <.001
Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test, mean (SD)4.90 (5.9)2.97 (3.3)t = −2.81 p = .006
PTSD Duration, mean (SD)8.02 (8.40)--
Serotonergic medication, # (%)24 (24)4 (4)χ 2 =17.3, p <.001
Antipsychotic medication, # (%)7 (7)0 (0)χ 2 =7.40, p = .007
Participants reported race and ethnicity information according to investigator-defined options.

Two participants in the PTSD group and two participants in the non-PTSD group were excluded (total n=4) due to use of mood stabilizers that have previously been reported to have conflicting effects on brain volume3637. PTSD duration, determined from clinical assessment and defined as the time elapsed between the occurrence of criterion-A trauma and the MRI scan, was available for 91 of 99 participants. For delayed onset of symptoms (n=4), ranging from eight months to one year, PTSD duration was calculated relative to the time of symptom onset.

MRI acquisition

Images were acquired on a General Electric 3 Tesla Signa EXCITE scanner equipped with an 8-channel headcoil. High-resolution T1-weighted whole brain images with 1-mm isotropic voxels using array spatial sensitivity encoding technique (ASSET) and fast spoiled gradient-recall (3D-FSPGR) were acquired axially for all participants. Image parameters were optimized for contrast between white matter, gray matter, and CSF (TR/TE/flip angle=7.484 ms/2.984 ms/12°, FOV=256 mm, 1-mm slice thickness, 166 slices, 256×256 matrix, 1 excitation).

Image analysis

All T1 images were visually inspected (CCH) to assure appropriate quality. Automated segmentation and labeling of the amygdala and hippocampus and estimation of total intracranial volume from participants’ T1 images were performed using the FreeSurfer image analysis suite38 (version 5.0.0; http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) and its library tool recon-all. We previously validated FreeSurfer automated segmentation of the amygdala and hippocampus compared to manual tracing27. Spatial normalization by affine registration to Talairach space and skull stripping were performed on the T1 images. Registration was checked visually for accuracy (CCH). FreeSurfer segmentation and labeling of subcortical structures was based on a combination of voxel intensity, probabilistic atlas location, and the spatial relationships of voxels to the location of nearby subcortical structures. Using the FreeSurfer library function mri_label2vol and a transformation matrix generated by tkregister2, the segmentation labels were returned to native space. The native-space segmentations were converted to LAS orientation and then the amygdala and hippocampus were extracted using the segmentation labels. The segmentation of the amygdala and hippocampus overlayed on original T1 images (see Figure 1) was visually inspected (ALG, VMB, KSL) slice-by-slice for correct location and shape. All participants passed this inspection process without the need for manual adjustment.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms452834f1.jpg

Example of automated segmentation of the amygdala and hippocampus using FreeSurfer. The high-resolution structural MRI of a representative subject is displayed in radiological convention with segmentation labels for the amygdala (in blue) and hippocampus (in yellow) as shown in (A) sagittal (left hemisphere), (B) coronal, and (C) axial slices.

Statistical analysis

We used the general linear model (GLM) to control for potential confounding variables in examining the influence of PTSD diagnosis on regional brain volume. GLM analysis (VMB, RAM, GM, biostatistician HRW) on each brain region (left and right amygdala and hippocampus) included the following covariates: intracranial volume, age, gender, combat exposure, traumatic lifetime events, depressive symptoms, alcohol abuse, duration of PTSD, and use of serotonergic (n=28) and antipsychotic (n=7) medications (separate covariates) based on reports of increased regional volumes associated with serotonergic39 and decreased regional volumes associated with antipsychotic40 agents. Alpha was .05 given our a priori hypotheses of PTSD volume differences in the amygdala and hippocampus.

Because of expected intercorrelations among the covariates, principal components analysis (PCA) was performed to reduce the data dimensionality. A principal components factor analysis using varimax rotation was conducted on age, PTSD duration, BDI, CES, TLEQ, and AUDIT scores. Selection of specific components to be used in follow-up modeling was confirmed by parallel analysis41, which compares the eigenvalues from the principal components analysis with those from a randomly generated dataset of the same size (Monte Carlo PCA for Parallel Analysis, http://edpsychassociates.com/Watkins3.html). Only the components with eigenvalues higher than the randomly generated dataset were retained and substituted for the individual covariates in follow-up modeling.

Finally, the disparity in trauma load between groups raised the possibility that differences in volume might be associated with magnitude of trauma exposure rather than PTSD illness. Therefore, we repeated the GLM on two subgroups that were matched for combat exposure and lifetime trauma.

RESULTS

Demographic Information

Basic demographic and clinical information is reported by diagnostic group in Table 2.

Volumetry results

Group means, standard deviations, and the GLM results testing our hypotheses on the effects of PTSD diagnosis, combat exposure, lifetime trauma, illness chronicity, and BDI are summarized in Table 3. The between-group results demonstrated that PTSD diagnosis was associated with smaller volume in the left and right amygdala and the left hippocampus. Right hippocampal volume was not significantly associated with PTSD diagnosis (see Table 3). Combat exposure was not significantly related to left amygdala, right amygdala, or right hippocampal volume, but showed a trend towards significance for left hippocampal volume. Lifetime trauma, illness chronicity, and depressive symptoms were not associated with volume differences in either structure for either hemisphere. Intracranial volume was significantly correlated with bilateral amygdala and hippocampus volumes (all p-values < .001). The other covariates including AUDIT, medication use, age, gender, BDI, CES, and PTSD duration were not significantly related to either amygdala or hippocampal volumes (data not shown; all p-values > .05). Results obtained by statistically controlling for medication effects were compared to a secondary analysis that excluded participants on antidepressant (n=28) and antipsychotic medications (n=7). Smaller volumes were confirmed in left amygdala [F(1,154)=8.67, p =. 004], right amygdala [F(1,154)=6.60, p =. 01], and left hippocampus [F(1,154)=5.74, p = .02]. The right hippocampus did not reach statistical significance [F(1,154)=1.89, p = .17].

Table 3

Volumetry Results by diagnosis and effect of trauma load and illness chronicity

Brain StructureVolume mm3 (mean ± SD)PTSDCombat Exposure ScaleTrauma Life Events QuestionnairePTSD DurationBeck Depression InventoryPTSD effect size a Cohen’s d
ControlPTSDF(1,180)pF(1,180)pF(1,180)pF(1,180)pF(1,180)p
AmygdalaL1810 ± 2311746 ± 23310.0.0022.26.131.22.272.78.102.08.15−.28
R1994 ± 2571894 ± 2576.83.0102.54.11.589.44.375.54.02.89−.39
HippocampusL4180 ± 5054067 ± 4215.81.023.60.06.139.711.01.321.68.20−.22
R4188 ± 4694129 ± 4151.36.251.23.27.005.951.24.27.73.39−.13

Duration of PTSD was unavailable in eight PTSD participants, therefore analysis of the whole group omitted this covariate to include 99 participants with PTSD and 101 controls (see Table 4). The differences in volume between PTSD and non-PTSD groups in the right amygdala and left hippocampus were significant but had a higher p-value than with the inclusion of PTSD duration as a covariate, whereas the volume difference in the left amygdala was at trend level. The volume difference in the right hippocampus was again not significant. Severity of PTSD in patients assessed with the CAPS, controlling for ICV, was negatively correlated with volume of the left amygdala (r = −.22, p = .03), but did not reach significance in the right amygdala (r = −.18, p = .09). As expected, removing ICV as a covariate resulted in a non-significant correlation given the established association between head size and regional brain volume.

Table 4

Effect of diagnosis after omitting PTSD duration from GLM

Brain StructureF(1,189)p-value
AmygdalaL3.55.06
R5.51.02
HippocampusL4.78.03
R.920.40

Data reduction

Two components, which individually explained 30.3% and 26.9% of the variance, respectively, had eigenvalues greater than 1. Selection of these two components was confirmed by parallel analysis41. The first component was associated with depressive symptoms, traumatic events, longer PTSD duration, older age, and combat exposure. The second component was associated with longer PTSD duration, younger age, alcohol abuse, and less combat exposure. A factor analysis that included intracranial volume showed it had low communality (.33), meaning the extracted components accounted for minimal ICV variance). Consequently, ICV was excluded from the final factor analysis. Results of GLM using covariates of components 1 and 2, intracranial volume, gender, use of serotonergic medication, and use of antipsychotic medication were similar to the original analyses conducted with all covariates. That is, PTSD diagnosis was associated with smaller left and right amygdala volume and left hippocampal volume (see Table 5).

Table 5

Results for diagnosis using component covariates

Brain StructureF(1,184)p-value
AmygdalaL6.27.01
R6.59.010
HippocampusL6.76.01
R1.35.25

Trauma Load

To account for the possible nonlinear influences of trauma exposure on regional brain volume irrespective of PTSD diagnosis, follow-up analysis conducted with subgroups matched for combat exposure and lifetime trauma exposure (n=122: 76 PTSD and 46 non-PTSD) revealed similar group differences to the primary analytic models (see Table 6). There were significant group differences in left and right amygdala, but a trend significance in the left hippocampus.

Table 6

Results of PTSD diagnosis matched for trauma exposure

Brain StructureF(1,110)p-value
AmygdalaL7.97.006
R6.54.02
HippocampusL3.56.07
R1.93.17

COMMENT

Our study establishes diminished amygdala volume in a large cohort of recent military veterans with PTSD compared to trauma-exposed non-PTSD veterans after controlling for depressive symptoms, alcohol use, intracranial volume, medication, PTSD chronicity, and trauma load. In contrast to inconclusive results from prior studies that reported marginal or non-significant effects for amygdala volumetry, we found a significant association between smaller amygdala volume and PTSD. This association was not accounted for by PTSD chronicity, trauma load, severity of depressive symptoms, alcohol use, or medication status. We confirmed findings of smaller volume in the left hippocampus for PTSD compared to trauma-exposed non-PTSD controls, which remained significant after controlling for the same variables used in the amygdala analysis. The laterality of our findings for hippocampus was consistent with a prior meta-analysis13 that showed significantly decreased left but not right hippocampal volume in PTSD.

Comparison to previous findings for amygdala volumetry

Although most previous studies reported non-significant findings, some studies showed a trend towards significance for smaller amygdala volumes in PTSD21,22,24,26,42. The investigations of PTSD and amygdala volume with the largest samples included PTSD groups with n=4443 and n=2844, but were conducted in children and therefore do not generalize well to adults due to developmental changes in brain structure and connectivity45. All studies of adults had a sample size of fewer than 20 in the PTSD group, with the majority of studies having 15 or fewer participants20,2226. Consistent with our findings and relevant to the core symptom cluster of re-experiencing symptoms in PTSD, smaller amygdala volume was associated with the presence of cancer-related intrusive recollections in a sample of 76 breast cancer survivors18.

It is worthwhile to consider the factors that may have produced a significant association of decreased amygdala volume given the preponderance of negative findings in prior studies. The small effect sizes observed from the meta-analyses suggest that they were underpowered to detect significant differences. Assessing a large sample size is impractical with manual segmentation, and this motivated our use of an automated segmentation technique. Automated segmentation also: (i) facilitates future replication of these results by other investigators; (ii) diminishes differences among studies due to the use of different protocols for defining anatomical boundaries; (iii) eliminates variability associated with different raters across studies and even a single rater over time; and (iv) removes bias introduced by varied software interfaces that are used in manual tracing (e.g., 3-plane views vs. single plane view). Thus, replication of the present results based on automated segmentation may be achieved with greater fidelity than manual tracing methods38. Although we previously reported that FreeSurfer introduces additional variance compared to the gold standard of hand tracing27,46, this variance would not bias volumetry measures in favor of one group over another.

Rather than matching individuals on a participant-by-participant basis to achieve the large enrollment necessary for this study in a realistic time frame, we used a statistical approach21 to control for potentially confounding variables (e.g., depressive symptoms, trauma load, duration of PTSD, intracranial volume, age, medication, alcohol abuse). The effects of many of these variables have not previously been tested as covariates in PTSD amygdala investigations. Assembling a PTSD group that is free of depressive symptoms is unlikely to generalize, and moreover new evidence calls into question whether PTSD and depression are distinct entities among individuals exposed to trauma47. Notably, there was no significant association between depressive symptoms and amygdala or hippocampal volumes after controlling for PTSD among the other covariates. Larger amygdala volumes have been found in early depression48 but smaller or null findings have been found in chronic depression49. Nevertheless, it is important to exert statistical control for these symptoms. Exclusion of participants taking psychotropic medication has also been the accepted orthodoxy, although leaders in the field of PTSD neuroimaging have argued for their inclusion50. Technical concerns in previous studies include the use of older MRI technology such as lower field strength scanners (1.5 or 2.0 Tesla, see Table 1)19,2124,4244 and lower spatial resolution (≥ 1.5 mm slice thickness)1926,4244. We addressed these issues with a 3 Tesla scanner to improve SNR and 1-mm slice thickness (1mm isometric) for superior spatial resolution. Based upon our previously reported power calculation for amygdala segmentation with FreeSurfer, a sample size of at least 55 subjects per group is required for the effect sizes we observed27.

In addition to these methodological improvements, important demographic and trauma-related characteristics of our sample differ from those of previous studies. In contrast to some earlier studies of veterans with chronic PTSD2324,51, our participants were recent military personnel who served largely in military conflicts in Iraq and Afghanistan.

Associations with functional neuroimaging of amygdala

Functional differences of the amygdala, particularly in the left hemisphere, during emotion processing were supported by a meta-analysis showing ventral anterior hyperactivation and a dorsal posterior hypoactivation in PTSD9. Amygdala engagement during fear conditioning is well established in healthy adults. Thus, amygdala hyperactivity in PTSD may reflect an exaggerated response of fear circuitry and may explain PTSD symptoms such as hypervigilance and hyperarousal. Despite concerns with statistical power, heterogeneity of task design, patient characteristics, imaging modality, and analytic approaches in functional neuroimaging studies of PTSD, these results have been more consistent than results from volumetric studies of the amygdala. Overall, there are numerous reports of greater amygdala activation in PTSD67,5255, whereas some others failed to show increased amygdala activation5658, further obfuscating a coherent hypothesis for amygdala volume differences in PTSD. Indeed, amygdala volume decrease appears to correspond to increased fMRI functional activation in PTSD59. The ventral hyperactive cluster reported by Etkin and Wager9 may relate to the basolateral amygdala (described below) and may be relevant to acquired fear responses in PTSD given the role of this region in forming emotional memories6061.

Pathobiology of PTSD

The amygdala plays a key role in a wide variety of behaviors and mnemonic functions, most critically in modulating negative affect and emotion.3, 63, 64 Changes in fear, stress, and anxiety in rats are induced by lesions to subnuclei of the amygdala, notably the BLA, which is needed to form and later express associative fear memories. Evidence primarily from rodent work has led to a model in which the lateral nucleus in the BLA receives aversive and sensory signals that are passed on to the basal nucleus and central nucleus. Fear-associated behaviors are governed by the central nucleus, which provides the major outputs of the amygdala.65,66 This model is consistent with the observation that prior to fear conditioning, rats with lesions to the lateral nucleus did not form fear memories, unlike lesions to the basal nucleus.67,68 However, following fear conditioning, lesions to the basal nucleus blocked the expression of fear memories, but not the ability to encode those memories.69 Thus, fear-related plasticity in the amygdala is essential for fear learning and accompanying fear behaviors.11,66

The effect of persistent and chronic threat-induced hyperexcitation of the amygdala on its volume in humans is yet unclear. Much of our knowledge on the pathobiology of threat and stress effects on the brain, and on the medial temporal lobe in particular, comes from animal models using acute stressors that are qualitatively different from the chronic and/or extreme stressors typically experienced by humans with PTSD. One of several competing theories is based on established findings that the amygdala and hippocampus undergo stress-induced structural remodeling, albeit in very different ways.10 Most of the research on stress effects has focused on the hippocampus, with very few studies investigating the amygdala directly.

Smaller amygdala volume: vulnerability or consequence?

The second goal of this study was to gain insight into whether the smaller amygdala volume is a preexisting vulnerability factor for developing PTSD or a consequence of having PTSD. Our data failed to show a correlation of trauma load or PTSD chronicity with lower amygdala volume, suggesting the lack of a dose-response effect for trauma and amygdala volume or that smaller amygdalae might be a risk factor for developing PTSD.

Evidence from prior work in humans and animals demonstrates an association between smaller amygdalae and stronger fear conditioning and stress reactivity, which are considered risk factors for PTSD. Prior work in animals by Yang and colleagues11 used recombinant inbred strains of mice that exhibit up to a 2-fold difference in BLA size.11 Mice were categorized into groups with small, medium, and large BLA volume and underwent a pavlovian fear-conditioning procedure. The small-BLA mice showed stronger fear conditioning than the medium- and large- BLA mice, and freezing to the conditioned stimulus was significantly correlated with volume of the BLA but not of comparison regions, including the hippocampus, striatum, or cerebellum. Mice were also subjected to a stress condition (forced swim), which was associated with elevated corticosterone level in the small-BLA but not the medium- or large-BLA groups. Nonstressed mice did not differ by corticosterone level. The BLA is a critical site through which corticosterone enhances associative fear memories.

The BLA volume association raises a fundamental question whether a small BLA is the consequence or the cause of stronger fear conditioning. A smaller BLA is unlikely to be a consequence given that chronic threat and stress lead to corticosterone-mediated spinogenesis and dendritic arborization in mice.10, 7072 In clear contradistinction, the hippocampus shows loss of neurons and synaptic connectivity in response to elevated adrenal hormone levels associated with chronic threat and stress.7375 Thus, the smaller volume of the BLA, which constitutes the largest of the amygdalar nuclei, is linked to stronger fear conditioning, chronic threat, and vigilance, leading to a potential increase in BLA volume.5 Although the effects of chronic threat and stress in rodents may differ in humans, the neural systems for fear processing are highly conserved across phylogeny. The evidence of counteracting influences on amygdala volume found in rodents is likely to cloud any association of amygdala volumetry and PTSD and may explain the inconsistent and sometimes conflicting reports of amygdala volumes in PTSD.14, 15

Evidence from 2 studies in humans builds on the work of Yang and colleagues11 in mice. Studies in humans not exposed to trauma or chronic threat and stress are especially informative because trauma-induced structural changes in the amygdala are unlikely to have occurred. First, Hartley et al 13 paired colored squares (conditioned stimuli) with mild electrical shock (unconditioned stimuli) and measured the strength of fear acquisition via skin conductance response. The magnitude of the conditioned fear response was correlated with the smaller amygdala volume. Second, Gianaros et al12 measured mean arterial pressure in response to a stressor (performance-titrated Stroop Color-Word Interference task) and found that a smaller amygdala volume was correlated with stressor-evoked blood pressure reactivity.

To investigate directly whether our finding of decreased amygdala volume in PTSD represents a preexisting vulnerability factor or an acquired sign of the disorder, research studies using prospective, longitudinal design and twin-discordance models are needed.76 Vulnerability related to genetic or epigenetic effects has been hotly debated, but little empirical evidence is available. Altered serotonin binding in the left amygdala8 and increased left amygdala activation modulation by the 5HTTLPR serotonin transporter gene77 have been observed in PTSD. However, evidence showing specific genetic modulation of amygdala volume in PTSD is lacking.

Limitations

We used covariates to control for variables such as depressive symptoms, alcohol abuse, and age. This approach is adequate unless a nonlinear association is present. For instance, trauma exposure may not follow a linear dose response but instead may require a specific threshold beyond which a marked effect on amygdala volume is produced. Secondary analyses that matched groups for trauma exposure were performed to rule out this possibility. Also, our sample consisted of veterans from the Iraq and Afghanistan conflicts, who were mostly men; therefore, owing to these and other sources of selection bias, we urge caution when generalizing these results to other demographic groups. Our sample also showed high levels of combat trauma relative to other trauma types, and it remains unclear whether the type of trauma predicts the magnitude of volume loss in the amygdala. Approximately one-fourth of the sample was diagnosed using the DTS, a self-report measure that has high predictive power compared with the CAPS but may misclassify some subjects. Finally, based on the effect sizes we obtained for the hippocampus, the present sample size may have been insufficient to detect volume differences in the right hippocampus; however, this effect size was consistent with a large meta-analysis.15

Conclusions

These results provide robust evidence of an association between a smaller amygdala volume and PTSD. We did not observe correlation between trauma load or illness chronicity with amygdala volume. When considered in the context of previous translational research linking smaller amygdala volume with stronger fear conditioning and stress response, our results are consistent with the theory that a smaller amygdala represents a vulnerability to developing PTSD rather than an outcome of the disorder. The story for amygdala volumetry might be more elusive than that of the hippocampus. For instance, counteracting influences show that, on one hand, smaller amygdala size may be consistent with a vulnerability to PTSD but that elevated corticosterone levels lead to increased amygdala volume on the other hand. Our results may trigger a renewed impetus for investigating structural changes in the amygdala, its genetic determinants, environmental modulators, and the possibility that lower amygdala volume represents an intrinsic vulnerability to PTSD.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health Grant K23 MH073091 (RAM), the Department of Veterans Health Affairs (VHA), GRANT00507153, GRANT10490938, and Mental Illness Research Education and Clinical Center Grant for Post-Deployment Mental Health.

Footnotes

Previous Presentation:

66th Annual Meeting - Society of Biological Psychiatry, San Francisco, CA on May 13, 2011.

The authors have no conflicts of interest and no financial disclosures. The entities that provided funding support had no input into the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript. The principal investigator (Rajendra Morey, M.D.) had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

References

1. Rauch SL, Shin LM, Phelps EA. Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research--past, present, and future. Biological Psychiatry. 2006 Aug 15;60(4):376–382. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
2. Dunsmoor JE, Prince SE, Murty VP, Kragel PA, LaBar KS. Neurobehavioral mechanisms of human fear generalization. Neuroimage. 2011 Apr 15;55(4):1878–1888. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
3. LaBar KS, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinction: a mixed-trial fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20(5):937–945. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
4. Pape HC, Pare D. Plastic synaptic networks of the amygdala for the acquisition, expression, and extinction of conditioned fear. Physiol Rev. 2010 Apr;90(2):419–463. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
5. McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: Central role of the brain. Physiol Rev. 2007 Jul;87(3):873–904. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
6. Morey RA, Dolcos F, Petty CM, Cooper DA, Hayes JP, LaBar KS, McCarthy G. The role of trauma-related distractors on neural systems for working memory and emotion processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2009 May;43(8):809–817. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
7. Shin LM, Wright CI, Cannistraro PA, Wedig MM, McMullin K, Martis B, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Cavanagh SR, Krangel TS, Orr SP, Pitman RK, Whalen PJ, Rauch SL. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005 Mar;62(3):273–281. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
8. Murrough JW, Huang Y, Hu J, Henry S, Williams W, Gallezot JD, Bailey CR, Krystal JH, Carson RE, Neumeister A. Reduced Amygdala Serotonin Transporter Binding in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2011 Aug 17; [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
9. Etkin A, Wager TD. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: A meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007 Oct;164(10):1476–1488. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
10. Roozendaal B, McEwen BS, Chattarji S. Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10(6):423–433. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
11. Yang RJ, Mozhui K, Karlsson RM, Cameron HA, Williams RW, Holmes A. Variation in mouse basolateral amygdala volume is associated with differences in stress reactivity and fear learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008 Oct;33(11):2595–2604. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
12. Gianaros PJ, Sheu LK, Matthews KA, Jennings JR, Manuck SB, Hariri AR. Individual differences in stressor-evoked blood pressure reactivity vary with activation, volume, and functional connectivity of the amygdala. J Neurosci. 2008 Jan 23;28(4):990–999. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
13. Hartley CA, Fischl B, Phelps EA. Brain structure correlates of individual differences in the acquisition and inhibition of conditioned fear. Cereb Cortex. 2011 Sep;21(9):1954–1962. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
14. Woon FL, Hedges DW. Amygdala volume in adults with posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 2009 Oct;21:5–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
15. Karl A, Schaefer M, Malta LS, Dorfel D, Rohleder N, Werner A. A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD. Neurosci Biobehav R. 2006;30(7):1004–1031. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
16. Woon FL, Sood S, Hedges DW. Hippocampal volume deficits associated with exposure to psychological trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: A meta-analysis. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2010 Oct 1;34(7):1181–1188. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
17. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. Academic Press; New York: 1985. [Google Scholar]
18. Matsuoka Y, Yamawaki S, Inagaki M, Akechi T, Uchitomi Y. A volumetric study of amygdala in cancer survivors with intrusive recollections. Biological Psychiatry. 2003 Oct 1;54(7):736–743. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
19. De Bellis MD, Hall J, Boring AM, Frustaci K, Moritz G. A pilot longitudinal study of hippocampal volumes in pediatric maltreatment-related disorder posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2001 Aug 15;50(4):305–309. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
20. Bonne O, Brandes D, Gilboa A, Gomori JM, Shenton ME, Pitman RK, Shalev AY. Longitudinal MRI study of hippocampal volume in trauma survivors with PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158(8):1248–1251. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
21. Bremner JD, Randall P, Vermetten E, Staib L, Bronen RA, Mazure C, Capelli S, McCarthy G, Innis RB, Charney DS. Magnetic resonance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse--a preliminary report. Biological Psychiatry. 1997 Jan 1;41(1):23–32. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
22. Fennema-Notestine C, Stein MB, Kennedy CM, Archibald SL, Jernigan TL. Brain morphometry in female victims of intimate partner violence with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2002 Dec 1;52(11):1089–1101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
23. Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, Kasai K, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK. Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nature Neuroscience. 2002;5(11):1242–1247. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
24. Gurvits TV, Shenton ME, Hokama H, Ohta H, Lasko NB, Gilbertson MW, Orr SP, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, McCarley RW, Pitman RK. Magnetic resonance imaging study of hippocampal volume in chronic, combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 1996;40(11):1091–1099. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
25. Lindauer RJL, Vlieger E-J, Jalink M, Olff M, Carlier IVE, Majoie CBLM, den Heeten GJ, Gersons BPR. Smaller hippocampal volume in Dutch police officers with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2004 Sep 1;56(5):356–363. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
26. Wignall EL, Dickson JM, Vaughan P, Farrow TFD, Wilkinson ID, Hunter MD, Woodruff PWR. Smaller hippocampal volume in patients with recent-onset posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2004 Dec 1;56(11):832–836. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
27. Morey RA, Petty CM, Xu Y, Hayes JP, Wagner HR, Lewis DV, Labar KS, Styner M, McCarthy G. A comparison of automated segmentation and manual tracing for quantifying hippocampal and amygdala volumes. Neuroimage. 2009 Apr;45(3):855–866. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
28. Dedert EA, Green KT, Calhoun PS, Yoash-Gantz R, Taber KH, Mumford MM, Tupler LA, Morey RA, Marx CE, Weiner RD, Beckham JC. Association of trauma exposure with psychiatric morbidity in military veterans who have served since September 11, 2001. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2009 Jun;43(9):830–836. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
29. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK, editors. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1996. [Google Scholar]
30. Kubany ES, Haynes SN, Leisen MB, Owens JA, Kaplan AS, Watson SB, Burns K. Development and preliminary validation of a brief broad-spectrum measure of trauma exposure: the Traumatic Life Events Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment. 2000 Jun;12(2):210–224. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
31. Lund M, Foy D, Sipprelle C, Strachan A. The Combat Exposure Scale: a systematic assessment of trauma in the Vietnam War. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1984 Nov;40(6):1323–1328. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
32. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption--II. Addiction. 1993 Jun;88(6):791–804. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
33. Blake DD, Weathers FW, Nagy LM, Kaloupek DG, Gusman FD, Charney DS, Keane TM. The development of a Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1995;8(1):75–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
34. Davidson JR, Book SW, Colket JT, Tupler LA, Roth S, David D, Hertzberg M, Mellman T, Beckham JC, Smith RD, Davison RM, Katz R, Feldman ME. Assessment of a new self-rating scale for post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychological Medicine. 1997 Jan;27(1):153–160. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
35. McDonald SD, Beckham JC, Morey RA, Calhoun PS. The validity and diagnostic efficiency of the Davidson Trauma Scale in military veterans who have served since September 11th, 2001. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2009 Mar;23(2):247–255. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
36. Phillips ML, Travis MJ, Fagiolini A, Kupfer DJ. Medication effects in neuroimaging studies of bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008 Mar;165(3):313–320. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
37. Savitz J, Nugent AC, Bogers W, Liu A, Sills R, Luckenbaugh DA, Bain EE, Price JL, Zarate C, Manji HK, Cannon DM, Marrett S, Charney DS, Drevets WC. Amygdala volume in depressed patients with bipolar disorder assessed using high resolution 3T MRI: The impact of medication. Neuroimage. 2010 Feb 15;49(4):2966–2976. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
38. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S, Montillo A, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002 Jan 31;33(3):341–355. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
39. Bremner JD, Vermetten E. Neuroanatomical changes associated with pharmacotherapy in posttraumatic stress disorder. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004 Dec;1032:154–157. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
40. Moncrieff J, Leo J. A systematic review of the effects of antipsychotic drugs on brain volume. Psychological Medicine. 2010 Sep;40(9):1409–1422. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
41. Pallant J. SPSS Survivial Guide: A step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS. 4. Berkshire, England: McGraw-Hill; 2010. [Google Scholar]
42. Rogers MA, Yamasue H, Abe O, Yamada H, Ohtani T, Iwanami A, Aoki S, Kato N, Kasai K. Smaller amygdala volume and reduced anterior cingulate gray matter density associated with history of post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Research-Neuroimaging. 2009 Dec 30;174(3):210–216. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
43. De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Clark DB, Casey BJ, Giedd JN, Boring AM, Frustaci K, Ryan ND. Developmental traumatology Part II: Brain development. Biological Psychiatry. 1999 May 15;45(10):1271–1284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
44. De Bellis MD, Keshavan MS, Shifflett H, Iyengar S, Beers SR, Hall J, Moritz G. Brain structures in pediatric maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder: A sociodemographically matched study. Biological Psychiatry. 2002 Dec 1;52(11):1066–1078. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
45. Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. The Structural and Functional Developmental Trajectory of the Frontal Cortex. Aust Nz J Psychiat. 2010 Sep;44:A7–A7. [Google Scholar]
46. Morey R, Selgrade ES, Wagner HR, Huettel SA, Wang L, McCarthy G. Scan-rescan reliability of subcortical brain volumes derived from automated segmentation. Human Brain Mapping. 2010 In press. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
47. Elhai JD, Carvalho LDF, Miguel FK, Palmieri PA, Primi R, Frueh BC. Testing whether posttraumatic stress disorder and major depressive disorder are similar or unique constructs. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2011 Apr;25(3):404–410. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
48. Lange C, Irle E. Enlarged amygdala volume and reduced hippocampal volume in young women with major depression. Psychol Med. 2004 Aug;34(6):1059–1064. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
49. Sheline YI, Gado MH, Price JL. Amygdala core nuclei volumes are decreased in recurrent major depression. Neuroreport. 1998 Jun 22;9(9):2023–2028. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
50. Lanius RA, Brewin CR, Bremner JD, Daniels JK, Friedman MJ, Liberzon I, McFarlane A, Schnurr PP, Shin L, Stein M, Vermetten E. Does neuroimaging research examining the pathophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder require medication-free patients? Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience. 2010 Mar;35(2):80–89. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
51. Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, Bronen RA, Seibyl JP, Southwick SM, Delaney RC, McCarthy G, Charney DS, Innis RB. MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1995;152(7):973–981. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
52. Hendler T, Rotshtein P, Yeshurun Y, Weizmann T, Kahn I, Ben-Bashat D, Malach R, Bleich A. Sensing the invisible: differential sensitivity of visual cortex and amygdala to traumatic context. Neuroimage. 2003 Jul;19(3):587–600. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
53. Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK. Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;47(9):769–776. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
54. Shin LM, Orr SP, Carson MA, Rauch SL, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Peters PM, Metzger LJ, Dougherty DD, Cannistraro PA, Alpert NM, Fischman AJ, Pitman RK. Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61(2):168–176. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
55. Williams LM, Kemp AH, Felmingham K, Barton M, Olivieri G, Peduto A, Gordon E, Bryant RA. Trauma modulates amygdala and medial prefrontal responses to consciously attended fear. Neuroimage. 2006 Jan 15;29(2):347–357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
56. Bremner J, Vermetter E, Vythilingam M, Afzal N, Schmahl C, Elzinga, Bernet, Charney D. Neural Correlates of the Classic Color and Emotional Stroop in Women with Abuse-Related Posttraumatic Stress Disoder. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;55:612–620. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
57. Clark CR, McFarlane AC, Morris P, Weber DL, Sonkkilla C, Shaw M, Marcina J, Tonchon-Danguy HJ, Egan GF. Cerebral Function in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder during Verbal Working Memory Updating: A Positron Emission Tomography Study. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;53:474–481. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
58. Shin LM, Whalen PJ, Pitman RK, Bush G, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Orr SP, McInerney SC, Rauch SL. An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;50(12):932–942. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
59. Ganzel BL, Kim P, Glover GH, Temple E. Resilience after 9/11: multimodal neuroimaging evidence for stress-related change in the healthy adult brain. Neuroimage. 2008 Apr 1;40(2):788–795. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
60. Jovanovic T, Ressler KJ. How the Neurocircuitry and Genetics of Fear Inhibition May Inform Our Understanding of PTSD. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010 Jun;167(6):648–662. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
61. McGaugh JL, Cahill L, Roozendaal B. Involvement of the amygdala in memory storage: Interaction with other brain systems. PNAS: Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1996;93:13508–13514. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
62. Davis M, Whalen PJ. The amygdala: Vigilance and emotion. Molecular Psychiatry. 2001 Jan;6(1):13–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
63. Phelps EA, LeDoux JE. Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron. 2005 Oct 20;48(2):175–187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
64. Fanselow MS, LeDoux JE. Why we think plasticity underlying pavlovian fear conditioning occurs in the basolateral amygdala. Neuron. 1999 Jun;23(2):229–232. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
65. Maren S, Quirk GJ. Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004 Nov;5(11):844–852. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
66. Goosens KA, Maren S. Contextual and auditory fear conditioning are mediated by the lateral, basal, and central amygdaloid nuclei in rats. Learn Mem. 2001 May-Jun;8(3):148–155. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
67. Koo JW, Han JS, Kim JJ. Selective neurotoxic lesions of basolateral and central nuclei of the amygdala produce differential effects on fear conditioning. J Neurosci. 2004 Sep 1;24(35):7654–7662. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
68. Quirk GJ, Anglada-Figueroa D. Lesions of the basal amygdala block expression of conditioned fear but not extinction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005 Oct 19;25(42):9680–9685. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
69. Vyas A, Mitra R, Shankaranarayana Rao BS, Chattarji S. Chronic stress induces contrasting patterns of dendritic remodeling in hippocampal and amygdaloid neurons. J Neurosci. 2002 Aug 1;22(15):6810–6818. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
70. Govindarajan A, Rao BSS, Nair D, Trinh M, Tonegawa S, Mawjee N, Chattarji S. Transgenic brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression causes both anxiogenic and antidepressant effects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006 Aug 29;103(35):13208–13213. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
71. Mitra R, Jadhav S, McEwen BS, Vyas A, Chattarji S. Stress duration modulates the spatiotemporal patterns of spine formation in the basolateral amygdala. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jun 28;102(26):9371–9376. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
72. Lupien SJ, de Leon M, de Santi S, Convit A, Tarshish C, Nair NP, Thakur M, McEwen BS, Hauger RL, Meaney MJ. Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits. Nature Neuroscience. 1998;1(1):69–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
73. Landfield PW, Baskin RK, Pitler TA. Brain aging correlates: retardation by hormonal-pharmacological treatments. Science. 1981 Oct 30;214(4520):581–584. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
74. Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS. The neuroendocrinology of stress and aging: the glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis. Endocr Rev. 1986 Aug;7(3):284–301. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
75. Pitman RK, Gilbertson MW, Gurvits TV, May FS, Lasko NB, Metzger LJ, Shenton ME, Yehuda R, Orr SP, Harvard VAPTSI. Clarifying the origin of biological abnormalities in PTSD through the study of identical twins discordant for combat exposure. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2006 Jul;1071:242–254. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
76. Morey RA, Hariri AR, Gold AL, Hauser MA, Munger HJ, Dolcos F, McCarthy G. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms and brain function during emotional distraction from cognitive processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. BMC Psychiatry. 2011;11:76. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Feedback